FT.com FT WEEKEND MAGAZINE - SCIENCE MATTERS: Sense of togetherness By Stephen Pincock, Financial Times Published: Jul 23, 2005 We've all had far too many opportunities over the past few years to contemplate being part of a crowd during a life-threatening emergency. For those in London, the nightmare has become particularly stark. As one of the millions of people who regularly ride the city's public transport system, I have found it almost impossible to avoid putting myself in the shoes of those caught up in the bombings. By odd coincidence, just days before the July 7 attacks, I had been talking to a group of British researchers who have been studying how people in crowds respond in emergencies. Before the Tube and bus bombs I thought the findings had been worth hearing - now they are strikingly relevant. John Drury and colleagues from the Universities of Sussex and St Andrews have been studying how crowds react in "panic" situations, and what factors influence the way individuals behave in such circumstances. The group interviewed a selection of people who had been involved in real-life emergencies, including the fatal crowd crushing at the Hillsborough football stadium in 1989, the fire at Bradford football stadium in 1985, the evacuation of Canary Wharf after September 11 and an extremely overcrowded concert by DJ Fatboy Slim on Brighton beach. Based on earlier research, Drury's team expected to find that there were two distinct sorts of crowd: one in which there was a history of unity, such as football crowds, where they expected more helping behaviour; and another in which people had been randomly thrown together, where they expected far less solidarity and more selfish behaviour. "In fact, what we found was that in the event of the emergency, people often described feeling an increased sense of togetherness or unity which grew in relation to the perceived threat of death," Drury said. "This then prompted more helping behaviour than you would have expected otherwise." To look into these unexpected observations further, the Sussex researchers got together with a team of computer scientists from Nottingham University, led by Damian Schofield, which specialises in creating models of disaster situations to help governments and companies prevent and deal with catastrophes. Drury and colleagues asked Schofield's group to create a simulation of a fire in the London Underground, in which a user has to travel through a station to escape. They wanted to use this experimental model to test the idea that a shared identity within a crowd is an important factor in promoting helping behaviour when people are faced with an emergency. At the Royal Society's summer science exhibition on July 4, I saw the simulator in action. On a computer screen, the view is as seen through the eyes of a person in crowded subway tunnel filled with people running away from a fire. Every now and then you come across an injured person lying on the floor and have to choose whether to help or to leave them behind. Meanwhile, an indicator shows a rising risk of death. "Don't think of this as a game," a volunteer was told. "Imagine what you would do in this situation." The computer model allows the scientists to manipulate how the participants define themselves - either as part of the crowd or psychologically distinct from it. "We ask people to play the role of, say, football fans who have all been to see a game, or that they've been shopping and are on their way home," explained Drury. After running the study on the first group of participants, Drury's group found that those who took part under the collective identity helped injured people on average 70 per cent of the time, compared to less than 50 per cent for those who were acting out the role of individuals in a random crowd. After a second round of simulations, they asked participants to fill in a questionnaire that probed how much they had felt a group identity within the crowd. Those who indicated they felt a strong sense of "groupness" were the most likely to give help, and this sense of identity was a better predictor of helping compared with factors such as the size of the crowd. No one is suggesting that everybody responds exactly the same way in an emergency, but the findings offer an insight into human behaviour that you could see echoed in the way Londoners responded to the bombings. Drury's group is now analysing accounts of people who were involved. (Anyone who would like share their experiences should go to www.cs.nott.ac.uk/~dzs/londonbomb/index.htm.) Drury also sees a potential lesson for officials. "What came out of the interviews was that this groupness sometimes takes time to develop as people gradually realise that everyone is in the same boat," he says. "There might be a role for the authorities in trying to promote this collective spirit in emergencies in the way they address the crowd. There need not be an assumption that people will panic rather than use the information in a logical way." stephen.pincock@journalist.co.uk